Localization of Function: Successes and Failures

Objective 4: State the major events that led to an understanding of the localization of function in the brain.

Therefore by the start of the 19th century, there was growing evidence for the Localization of Function hypothesis: contrary to Flourens’ belief, specific areas of the brain were specialized for specific functions. The story of neuroscience in the 1800s is really a steady advancement in our understanding of the localization of specific brain functions, culminating in Korbinian Brodmann’s famous map of the human brain, published in 1909 but still in use today.

We will look at the discoveries which led to the localization of four important brain functions:

  • personality
  • language
  • movement
  • vision

Plaque in Cavendish, Vermont commemorating the accident which changed Phineas Gage's life.On September 13, 1848, a foreman named Phineas Gage was supervising a crew of workmen who were building a railroad line near Cavendish, Vermont. The first American passenger railroad, the Baltimore and Ohio, had been started just 18 years before this, and the United States was in the grip of a railroad-building mania which would reach its peak with the driving of the “golden spike” joining the Union Pacific and Central Pacific railroad lines in 1869.

Map of New England showing location of Cavendish, Vermont.The area around Cavendish was predominantly flint rock. At the time, dynamite had not been invented, and so if rocks needed to be cleared, the best way was to drill a hole by hand, put a fuse and gunpowder into the resulting hole, cover it with sand to prevent sparks from setting off the charge prematurely, and tamp the whole assembly down with an iron tamping rod 3 cm in diameter and 109 cm long. The tamping iron had one pointy end and one blunt end; the blunt end was used to tamp the charge. The crew foreman was in charge of tamping the charge. Then everyone would go hide behind some rocks, the fuse was lit, and the charge would explode, breaking the rocks into small enough pieces to be moved by the crew.

Animation of Phineas Gage's injury.
Database Center for Life Science (DBCLS) Ratiu P, Talos IF, Haker S, Lieberman D, Everett P. The tale of Phineas Gage, digitally remastered. J Neurotrauma, 2004

On this day, something went horribly wrong. As Gage bent over the hole to tamp the charge, the iron tamping rod rocketed up through Gage’s left cheek and eye and out the top of his skull, and clattered a few dozen meters away on the rocks they were moving.

Miraculously, Gage survived. He retained all his ability to feel and move and think, except for one major change. His friends said, “Gage is no longer Gage.” His personality had changed irrevocably. A local physician, John Martyn Harlow, treated Gage’s head wound and wrote a paper with the beautifully simple title “Passage of an Iron Rod Through the Head” published in the Daguerrotype of Phineas Gage holding the iron rod that went through his head.Boston Medical and Surgical Journal in December 1848.

The change in Gage’s personality indicated that the frontal cortex, especially that part above the eye called the prefrontal cortex, is essential for the development and maintenance of our personality and the way we interact with others.

 

Photograph of Paul Broca.In 1861, French neurologist and neurosurgeon Paul Broca encountered a patient who had once carried the name Louis Victor Leborgne. In 1840, at age 30, his lifelong epileptic seizures had finally rendered him unable to speak except for one syllable. The hospital staff took to calling him “Monsieur Tan” because “tan” was the only word sound he could make; he usually said “tan tan”. Leborgne had been hospitalized for 21 years, since he had no wife or children to care for him, but on April 11, 1861, his paralyzed limbs had become gangrenous and he was transferred to Docteur Broca’s ward.

 

Leborgne's brain obtained at autopsy.In those days before antibiotics, not much could be done to combat gangrene, and Leborgne died a week later, on April 17, 1861. Broca obtained his brain for an autopsy and found a striking change in the left frontal cortex: a massive atrophic area. Broca then began collecting brains from similarly situated patients who had died after losing some aspect of the ability to speak. In each and every one, he found a similar atrophic area in the same location. This location, which would 40 years later be called area 44 and area 45 on Brodmann’s map, is also called Broca’s area in honor of Broca’s insight. Broca’s area is found on the left side of the brain in about 7/8 people and on the right side of the brain in about 1/8 people. Having Broca’s area on the right is strongly associated with left-handedness; about 1/4 of left-handed people have Broca’s area on the right. Not just speech, but the production of symbolic language (for example, writing) is located in Broca’s area, and when patients have a stroke or other injury to this area, just like Leborgne, they are unable to speak or their speech is severely impaired. This condition is called aphasia.

 

License

Icon for the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License

Introduction to Neuroscience Copyright © by Jim Hutchins; Lindsey Aune; and Rachel Jessop is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License, except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book